-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 0
/
Copy pathmanuscript.Rmd
230 lines (137 loc) · 30.3 KB
/
manuscript.Rmd
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
---
title : "Segmenting words and stems in an artificial language"
shorttitle : "Segmenting words and stems"
author:
- name : "Georgia Loukatou"
affiliation : "1"
corresponding : yes # Define only one corresponding author
address : "XXX"
email : "XXX"
- name : "Alejandrina Cristia"
affiliation : "2"
address : "29, rue d’Ulm, 75005 Paris, France"
email : "alecristia@gmail.com"
affiliation:
- id : "1"
institution : "Stanford University"
- id : "2"
institution : "Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et de Psycholinguistique, Département d'Etudes Cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL University"
author_note: >
TO ADD
abstract: >
How do learners break up the spoken stream into smaller, recombinable units? We investigate segmentation among adults exposed to artificial languages. Words in these languages are composed of stems and affixes, and thus both words and stems are potential segmentation units. We ask whether adults segment out stems, and whether they perform better on a stem or a word segmentation task. During the exposure phase, adult participants (N = 52; age > 18 years) watch videos of animals performing simple intransitive actions, while hearing sentences that describe those scenes. During a subsequent test phase, participants are given a two-alternative forced choice designed to assess the acceptability of words versus part-words, and stems versus part-stems.
keywords : "TO ADD"
wordcount : "~XXX words"
bibliography : ["nwrbib.bib"]
figsintext : yes
figurelist : no
tablelist : no
footnotelist : no
lineno : yes
lang : "en-US"
class : "man"
output :
papaja::apa6_pdf:
latex_engine: xelatex
---
```{r setup, include=FALSE}
REDOALL=FALSE #use true if you want to recalculate the final-data and other data sheets
# Seed for random number generation
set.seed(42)
knitr::opts_chunk$set(cache.extra = knitr::rand_seed)
options(knitr.graphics.error = FALSE)
#Sys.setlocale("LC_CTYPE", "en_US_POSIX.UTF-8")
#Sys.setlocale("LANG", "en_US_POSIX.UTF-8")
Sys.setlocale(category = "LC_ALL", locale = "en_SE.utf8")
knitr::opts_chunk$set(warning = FALSE, message = FALSE, cache = FALSE, fig.pos = "T")
#install.packages("devtools")
#devtools::install_github("crsh/papaja")
require("papaja")
library(kableExtra)
library(gdata)
library(tidyverse)
library(lme4)
library(openxlsx)
library(sjPlot)
library(stringr)
printr=function(x) sub("0.", ".", round(x,3))
p.levels <- c(0, 0.001, 0.01)
pval.display <- function(p.est) {
level <- findInterval(p.est, p.levels)
p.string <- ifelse(level == 3, paste0("= ", round(p.est, 2)),
ifelse(level == 2, "< 0.01", "< 0.001"))
return(p.string)
}
```
## TODO
- add the refs already cited
- flesh out the sections we already discussed
- perhaps reorganize intro slightly around two strands of literature
- draft results
- draft discussion
## Introduction
Many researchers believe that segmentation, the process whereby meaningful units are identified from running speech, is an important task when learning a language [saffran, frank, etc]. Studying this process is difficult in natural language, because learning the form and meaning of such smaller units may benefit from both perception and production experience, particularly in adulthood. In this study, we aim to shed light on the way that adult learners segment fluent sentences by exposing them to an artificial language with a complex morphological structure, in a design that builds on two lines of research introduced next: Learning of forms, and learning of grammatical units.
Both lines of work use artificial languages, created by the researchers. It is assumed that some learning mechanisms are shared between artificial and natural language learning (Gómez & Gerken, 2000; Reber, 1967). When used and interpreted properly, artificial languages can help obtain better experimental control over the input to which learners are exposed (Fedzechkina et al., 2016; Folia et al., 2010), and isolate specific learning factors (Hayakawa et al., 2020), especially for segmentation.
### Learning of forms
In this line of work, the accent is on how people cut up fluent speech into units that are then available in memory, in a sort of lexicon. The artificial languages used tend to be relatively simple, having only a handful of words and clear rules for between-word transitions, and are presented unimodally, typically with no meaning associated to the sentences. For instance, in Saffran et al. (1996), adults were exposed to an artificial language consisting of six trisyllabic words. The only cues available for word segmentation were the transitional probabilities between syllables (transitional probabilities between syllables spanning a word boundary were lower). The words were concatenated into fluent paragraphs and word boundaries were removed. The participants heard the text read by a speech synthesizer, with no acoustic word (or sentence) boundary cues, with an exposure lasting 21 minutes. At test, they were presented with the trisyllabic words and matched foils in a two-alternative forced choice. A preference for the words over the foils was interpreted as evidence that the participants had culled out the sequences from running speech, and thus learned the forms of the words.
The current standard for artificial language studies on segmentation is to focus on words as the target level of segmentation (e.g., Cunillera et al., 2010; Finn & Hudson Kam, 2008; Frank et al., 2013; Karuza et al., 2013; Kurumada et al., 2013; Saffran et al., 1997; Thiessen & Erickson, 2013; Tyler & Cutler, 2009 - but see segmentation of multi-word units by Siegelman & Arnon, 2015).\footnote{Numerous studies address segmentation and morpheme processing in infants and young children. Since the experiments in the present study bear only on adults, for whom learning mechanisms may be to a certain extent different from those found in children, we do not review that extensive literature.} In most such studies, the words composing the artificial language have no sub-units (stems or affixes) that could be found within other words. Participants seem to be able to segment words out of artificial languages based on different cues (transitional probabilities cues, Saffran et al., 1996; mapping to word referents, Cunillera et al., 2010). Moreover, segmented units seem to be available for other linguistic processes. For instance, adult learners generalise non-adjacent dependencies (Frost & Monaghan, 2016), map the word forms to word referents (Cunillera et al., 2010) and learn the overall linguistic structure (Siegelman & Arnon, 2015). There has been an effort to investigate word segmentation in artificial languages with structures that resemble more those of natural languages (e.g. variable word length and frequency, Frank et al., 2010; Hoch et al., 2013; Kurumada et al., 2013; Schuler et al., 2017). One important feature of human languages is that words may be composed of smaller meaningful units -- such as stems and affixes. Words’ and stems’ edges coincide in English much of the time, but not in many other languages. For example, in Modern Greek, adjective or verb stems can never be bare (without affixes). The verb κοιμάμαι, is composed by the verb stem κοιμα (which means “sleep”) and the suffix μαι indicating the first person singular. The noun μάγοι is composed by the noun stem μάγ (which means “magician”) and the affix οι indicating plural number and male gender. Incidentally, both affixes μαι and οι could also be found as free words in speech, because they are homophones with a preposition and a determiner, respectively. The fact that some stems never appear bare without other affixes is a cross-linguistically frequent feature, and could also be captured by artificial languages, and discovered by listeners when segmenting meaningful units. However, we are unaware of a study specifically testing whether listeners do segment out stems separately from affixes in this kind of study.
## Learning of grammatical units
In a different line of research focused on learning grammatical units, artificial languages tend to be more complex than in the other line of research summarized above, and participants typically are exposed to the language multimodally, with visual scenes representing the content of segmented sentences (e.g., Finley & Newport, 2010; Finley & Wiemers, 2013; Hudson Kam & Newport, 2009). For example, in Hudson Kam & Newport, 2009, participants were presented with a language having over 30 nouns, 12 verbs, a negation particle, and two different determiners, which were the main focus of the study, and which appeared in the order determiner-noun. Participants learned the language over 8 sessions, each over 25 minutes long, by listening to each sentence while watching the associated video, and then repeating the sentence. The test session contained vocabulary tests covering the language's nouns, as well as a sentence completion test, where the participant watched an event video and heard the main verb, having to produce the rest of the sentence. Results revealed that participants had been able to segment noun stems from the preceding determiners, even though exposure had been to full sentences, with no breaks between words.
Other studies in this line of work reveal that adults perform such segmentation even for separating stems and affixes ...
TO CONTINUE: the grammatical units are & generalization when shown systematic paradigms & stimuli where word boundaries are salient). mention L2 learning, decomposition, & maybe production
TODO (explain that what is missing is crossing of the 2 lits: formation of a "long term lexicon" via segmentation from fluent speech as in first line of work, & segmentation of meaningful & grammatical parts as in second line of work) Stems, like words, are building blocks of language and the ability to extract them is fundamental. Nonetheless, it is still unclear whether humans focus on bigger (words) or smaller (stems) blocks, or whether they process both levels in a complementary way. Despite the wealth of evidence that stems may also be readily segmented, no previous study has looked at stem segmentation, even though the aforementioned literature strongly suggests that humans should process both words and stems in language learning. Consequently, it is still unclear whether humans can segment stems out of running speech in an artificial language, and whether, if exposed to a language with both words and stems, they would succeed better in segmenting one or the other.
how do we learn subword units? is it only a secondary/deeper task? chunking/synthesis (REF) versus splitting/analysis (REF)? the role of items found in isolation? If we find that there is no difference in performance, perhaps there is no difference in representation: discussion regarding decomposition of items that are represented in the lexicon (Embick).
## This study
The goal of this study is to inform questions concerning the relationship between language segmentation and language structure, by investigating segmentation in two meaningful unit levels, words and stems, among adults exposed to artificial languages. Participants hear sentences of an artificial language, where words contain two or three morphemes. Their preference for words versus non-words and for stems versus non-stems will then be measured. The languages were designed to resemble natural languages such that content words are composed by stems and affixes. For the same reason, words were embedded in sentence frames, with more than one word each sentence. Transitional probabilities between syllables are controlled for, as explained in the Methods section. QUESTIONS AND PREDICTIONS MAKE REF TO 2AFC, EXPLAIN One crucial difference with respect to previous studies is that participants are not based on transitional probabilities or other such cues (e.g. length, frequency) to choose the correct answer, as these cues are controlled for (both words/stems and non-words/non-stems in each test trial have the same cues)..
Specifically, three key questions and predictions addressed in this study are:
1. Do participants segment out whole words in a language where there are affixes? If participants segment out words, then they will choose words more than non-words -- items that are not structurally words or stems. We predict that participants will do so, because word segmentation has been successful in previous artificial language studies where participants were presented with full sentences and because participants strip affixes in structure learning studies. However, it is possible that participants will fail to do so in our experiment, which combines the difficulty of complex morphology with passive exposure to unsegmented sentences. Should this occur, we can run a control in which participants are exposed to an artificial language that does not have systematically complex words.
2. Do participants segment out stems? If participants segment out stems, then they will choose stems more than non-stems -- items that are not structurally words or morphemes. We predict that participants will do so, because of the structure learning studies summarized above. However, if participants do not show a preference for stems over foils in this study, but they do show a preference for words over matched foils, this may indicate that stem segmentation occurs later and/or requires more exposure and/or requires more active manipulation of the language material. Before concluding so, we should consider our next question.
3. Which units, words or stems, are better segmented? To test this question, we will compare the two preference tests mentioned above in terms of preference. If results are stronger for words, then this may provide initial support for "analysis" theories; if results are stronger for stems than words, then this would be consistent with "synthesis" theories. It is also possible that there is no difference between the two, which would be consistent with the idea that words and stems are processed similarly.
The study is preregistered in OSF: https://osf.io/fuydc. We report fully on the design and planned analyses, and report results for one pilot.
## Methods
All materials and scripts can be found in https://osf.io/pzcs2/?view_only=4167d91834b34ba6bc84618c294a35bc.
### Stimuli
Each participant is assigned to one of four artificial languages, A, B, C or D. The languages were generated using different orders of concatenation of 18 syllables that are meaningless in French ("glu", "sin", "ga", "kli", "ten", "ko", "blu", "tun", "man", "blo", "ti", "gle" ,"da", "pun", "go", "kan", "fen" and "bi"). Each syllable only has one use in the vocabulary. A counterbalancing procedure was used to create the languages, as follows.
Language A: Syllables "glu", "sin", "ga", "kli" and "ten" form three noun stems, the next five syllables "ko", "blu", "tun", "man" and "blo" form three verb stems, syllables "ti", "gle" form the optional elements, "da", "pun" form the noun and verb singular, "go", "kan" form the plural affixes and "fen" and "bi" form the aspect affixes.
Language B was generated by inverting syllable order: syllables "bi", "fen","kan", "go", "pun" were used for the noun stems, "da", "gle","ti", "blo","man" for the verb stems, “"tun", "blu" for the optional elements, “ko”, “ten” for the noun and verb singular, “kli”, “ga” for plural and “sin”, “glu” for aspect).
Language C was created by inverting the order of syllables from the middle to the beginning, and then from the end to the middle: "blo", "man", "tun", "blu", "ko" form the noun stems, "ten", "kli", "ga", "sin", "glu" the verb stems, "bi" and "fen" the optional elements, "kan" and "go" the singular, "pun" and "da" the plural, and "gle" and "ti" the aspect.
Language D was created by starting from the middle to the end, and then from the beginning to the middle: "blo", "ti", "gle", "da", "pun" were used for noun stems, "go", "kan", "fen", "bi", "glu" for verb stems, "sin", "ga" for optional elements, "kli", "ten" for singular, "ko", "blu" for plural and "tun", "man" for aspect.
TODO ADD FIGURE 1
CAPTION: Utterance structure
Language elements were assigned to meanings, such that in each language one noun stem meant “dog”, another “cat”, another “sheep”; one verb stem meant “walk” , another “jump”, another “flip”. One noun affix indicated singular, another plural. One verb affix indicated singular, another plural, another a continuous action that extends over time and another an instantaneous action. At this point, then, items are akin to dictionary entries, having a phonological form and a meaning.
These items were then combined into sentences. The structure of each sentence is portrayed in Figure 1. Each sentence consists of a noun stem with its noun number affix and of a verb stem with an aspect affix and a verb number affix. The sentence can have one optional element at the beginning, or a different optional element at the end (akin to an adverb because it can occur at the beginning or the end, but having no meaning).
There were 96 sentences in each language generated for the training phase. The full compositionality is 3 noun one-or-two syllable stems x 2 noun number affixes x 3 verb one-or-two syllable stems x 2 aspect affixes x 2 number affixes. The same sentences may appear with no disrupters, one opening disrupter or one closing disrupter. Some sentences are held out, i.e. are logically possible but not presented. This is so that it can allow for certain requirements to be respected during the test phase, mainly item frequency and transitional probabilities between syllables, as will be presented in the next paragraph. In addition, we generated 14 ‘false’ items for the test phase, to be paired with ‘correct’ stem or whole word items. A correct noun word is a noun stem with an affix indicating number. A correct verb word is a verb stem with an affix indicating aspect and an affix indicating number. Stems are presented bare. The false items include fragments of sentences heard during the train phase, which do not form a stem or an entire word. These fragments may be a verb stem or the second syllable of a verb stem paired with only an aspect affix, the second syllable of a noun stem paired with a noun number affix, a noun number affix paired with a verb stem, or a noun number affix paired with a verb stem and an aspect affix. In total, the test phase consists of 30 paired-forced-choice trials. Fourteen trials have word and non-word pairs (six nouns versus non-nouns + eight verbs versus non-verbs). Sixteen trials have stem and non-stem pairs (eight noun stems versus non-noun stems + eight verb stems versus non-verb stems).
One requirement is having the same sum of forward transitional probabilities (TPs) between syllables of word and non-word items, as well as stem and non-stem items that will be paired in a test trial. Statistical learning using TPs is a primary source of evidence for segmentation in laboratory experiments for infants (Estes & Lew-Williams, 2015; R. L. Gomez, 2012; Pelucchi et al., 2009; Romberg & Saffran, 2010) and adults (Frank et al., 2010; Perruchet & Desaulty, 2008; Toro et al., 2005). In those studies, participants segment out words based on TPs.
However, since in this experiment we do not test the well-established use of TPs in segmentation, the TP information should be controlled for in the test. To this end, we adjusted the combination of stems and affixes during training. Stems have TP=1. When comparing a stem to a non-stem, some meaningless pair of syllables appearing next to each other should also have TP=1. This happens by presenting some stem always with the same affix. For example, the noun stem (‘sheep’) appears systematically in plural, and the verb stem (‘flip’) appears systematically with a non-progressive affix. For these stems, TP between stem and affix equals 1. One test example presents the participant with a choice between the correct verb “walk” (one-syllable verb stem + progressive affix + verb plural affix) and a false item: plural noun affix + one-syllable verb stem + progressive affix. Both choices are equally frequent in the training set, and TPs between syllables are 0.5. Another test includes a choice between the noun stem “sheep” (including a two-syllable noun stem) and a false choice, the second syllable of the noun stem and a noun plural affix. Both choices are equally frequent in the training set, and TPs between syllables are 1.
The text was converted to speech using the mac Speech Synthesis tool. Specifically, the Mexican voice “Paulina” was used because the voice has a flat prosody to avoid accidental insertion of prosodic cues. The speed of speech was fixed to 140 words per minute after pilot listening revealed that the default speed for this voice was very slow.
For the training phase, the audio sentences were paired with videos of scenes, which represent the meaning of the sentence. For example, if a sentence contains a ‘dog’ noun stem, and a ‘walk’ verb stem, the video would show a dog walking. The noun stem is followed by a number suffix, and the video shows one or two dogs. The verb stem is followed by an aspect suffix, and the video shows a dog walking continuously or once. Scenes of the videos are pictured in Figure 2. Videos are not shown during the test phase, and stimuli are presented purely auditorily.
TODO ADD FIGURE
Figure 2. Video samples from the study. In the first video, a cat walks. In the second video, two cats are walking.
### Procedure
The experiment was administered online, using the Labvanced Platform. There are two phases, which we call training and test. The training stimuli (audiovideos) are presented as utterances with clearly marked utterance boundaries, as cued both by silence and the end of a scene. The participant listens to a sentence while watching a video portraying the action described in the sentence. Participants are not given the sentence in writing, nor any breakdown of the different words, nor stems versus suffixes.
During the training period, we ensure attention through the use of catch trials. Eight questions testing the attention of the participant were added throughout the training period. Four questions ask which animal was seen in the last video. Three options are proposed that the participant has to choose from: a dog, a cat and a sheep. Four more questions ask which action was performed by the animal in the last video. Three options are proposed: walk, jump and flip.
Once the training period is over, participants are asked the following: ‘Vous allez maintenant entendre 2 sons. Quel son ressemble le plus à la langue que vous venez d'entendre? Ne réfléchissez pas trop et allez-y avec votre instinct!" (“You are now going to hear two sounds, which one sounds better for the language you just heard? Don’t overthink it and follow your gut”). They then need to press a button after hearing each trial: the left button if they think that the first item works better, or the right button for the second sound. Participants first perform a short mock test demonstrating the test process. They have to choose between pairs of french words, were one word is very frequent, and the other one is very rare.\footnote{The frequencies were retrieved from http://www.lexique.org/, a database of 140,000 French words and their information collected from diverse corpora (New et al, 2007).} The participants are asked which sound resembles more to the French language, and they can continue with the real test once they have answered correctly (selected the most frequent word) in two successive trials. They then perform the test trials.
At each trial, the participant only hears the stimuli, and thus there is no video. One can say that each trial presents them with a "correct" option and a foil. The correct option is a correctly segmented word or stem; the foil is an item that spans a word and/or stem boundary. Each test item has at least 2 syllables, to avoid missing or mishearing a (very short) sound. Paired items have the same length, but also result in the same sum of (forward) Transitional Probabilities, and have the same frequency (or, rarely, the foil has a larger frequency than the correct option). This way, no other cue could affect the preference of one versus the other item, other than the preference for a correctly segmented unit versus a non-unit. The whole experiment lasts 25 minutes. The study was initially designed to be tested in the lab, however, due to the current sanitary situation, it was updated for online testing.
Prior to the onset of the COVID-19 crisis, two pilot studies were conducted, each with eleven participants. Participants in the first pilot brought up three issues; the successive presentation of similar sounds during the test phase, the length of the familiarization period, and the absence of marked difference in verb aspect in the videos, e.g. they considered the ‘walk’ and ‘walking’ videos as describing the exact same action. These issues were successfully addressed in a second pilot. First, the repetition of the same stimulus or of a stimulus with the same stems is now avoided in successive trials. Second, an engaging message to the participant appears in the screen after completing 25%, 50% and 75% of the training, congratulating them for completing the corresponding part of the study. Third, the duration of the action in the videos with progressive actions was increased, and a "narrator" figure was added next to the videos.
The online version of the experiment was tested by four participants. Based on their feedback, the following two extra steps were taken when transferring the experiment online. First, eight questions testing the attention of the participant were added throughout the training period. Four questions ask which animal was seen in the last video. Three options are proposed that the participant has to choose from: a dog, a cat and a sheep. Four more questions ask which action was performed by the animal in the last video. Three options are proposed: walk, jump and flip. No feedback is given on the correctness of the answer. Second, right before the test section, the participants take a short mock test demonstrating the test process. They have to choose between pairs of french words, were one word is very frequent, and the other one is very rare. The participants are asked which sound resembles more to the French language, and they can continue with the real test, once they have answered correctly (selected the most frequent word) in two successive trials.
### Participants
There were 52 participants (XX male), aged XX years (range XX-XX), and having French as their native language based on self report. Participants were recruited with the Prolific crowd-sourcing platform (www.prolific.co) and the RISC participant platform (www.risc.cnrs.fr/). They were paid 10 euros for their participation.
We estimated that 52 participants would be sufficient, given that previous similar studies using artificial language segmentation experiments with adults found and average effect size of 0.46 in terms of the word versus foil two-alternative forced choice (Cunillera et al., 2010; Frost & Monaghan, 2016; Hoch et al., 2013; Perruchet & Desaulty, 2008; Toro et al., 2005; Tyler & Cutler, 2009 - a table with effect sizes and their average can be found in Supplementary material).
Eligible for inclusion were participants 1. who completed the experiment (meaning that they answered all test questions), 2. who answered correctly at least 4 of the attention questions, and their proportion of correct answers is not lower than two standard deviations from the mean group proportion, 3. whose average decision time during test was not lower than two standard deviations from the group average. Given these exclusion criteria, we continued recruitment until we achieved the required size, replacing participants with matching conditions as needed.
## Statistical Analysis
For the analysis, we will fit a generalized linear mixed effect model using the lme4 library in R (R studio team, 2015). The participants’ answers (right/wrong) will be the dependent variable and the level (stem/word) will be a fixed variable. The random effect of the participant is included, with level and number of trial (1st, 2nd…) as random slopes.
Pilot Data
Participants of the second pilot adapted well to the particularities of the complex linguistic structure, and considered both minimal meaningful units and words when listening to this unknown language. On average, 64% of their responses on word recognition (SD=0.186) and 61% of their responses on stem recognition (SD=0.16) were correct.
With respect to the three key questions, we observed that: 1. based on the intercept in a regression with the word level as baseline, the word trial coefficient was 0.62 (SE=0.28), with p-value=0.024. 2. based on the intercept of a regression with the stem level as baseline, the stem trial coefficient was 0.47 (SE=0.342), with p-value=0.003. 3. Based on the level as a fixed effect in a regression with the stem level as baseline, the coefficient for level is 0.162 (SE=0.248, p-value=0.540). Thus, recognition of words versus stems is not significantly different in this pilot. The trend is for more accurate responses for word trials than stem trials.
In a further exploratory analysis, we asked whether there is a difference in segmentation between verbs and nouns. The two word classes may not be overall processed in a similar way. Verbs could be more difficult to segment than nouns, for example due to the presence of more morphemes per word on average. We investigated this by including type (noun /verb) as a fixed effect variable and as a random slope. In the pilot results, the type coefficient was -0.33 (SE=0.239, p-value=0.169). The trend was for more accurate responses for nouns than verbs, but the result was non-significant.
The pilot results establish the feasibility of the proposed experiment.
## Results
## Discussion
\newpage
## Acknowledgments
AC acknowledges financial and institutional support from Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR-16-DATA-0004 ACLEW, ANR-14-CE30-0003 MechELex, ANR-17-EURE-0017) and the J. S. McDonnell Foundation Understanding Human Cognition Scholar Award.
## Data, code and materials availability statement
All data, code, and materials are available from [https://osf.io/5qspb/](https://osf.io/5qspb/)
# References
\setlength{\parindent}{-0.5in}
\setlength{\leftskip}{0.5in}