-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 0
/
Copy pathpages.txt
5038 lines (5033 loc) · 458 KB
/
pages.txt
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
I feel myself a naturalist and a physician both; and that I am equally interested in diseases and
people; perhaps, too, that I am equally, if inadequately, a theorist and dramatist, am equally drawn to the
scientific and the romantic, and continually see both in the human condition, not least in that
quintessential human condition of sickness—animals get diseases, but only man falls radically into
sickness. My work, my life, is all with the sick—but the sick and their sickness drives me to thoughts which, perhaps, I might otherwise not have. So much so that I am compelled to ask, with Nietzsche: ‘As for
sickness: are we not almost tempted to ask whether we could get along without it?’—and to see the questions it raises as fundamental in nature. Constantly my patients drive me to question, and constantly
my questions drive me to patients—thus in the stories or studies which follow there is a continual
movement from one to the other.
Studies, yes; why stories, or cases? Hippocrates introduced the historical conception of disease, the
idea that diseases have a course, from their first intimations to their climax or crisis, and thence to their
happy or fatal resolution. Hippocrates thus introduced the case history, a description, or depiction, of the
natural history of disease—precisely expressed by the old word ‘pathology.’ Such histories are a form of
natural history—but they tell us nothing about the individual and his history; they convey nothing of the
person, and the experience of the person, as he faces, and struggles to survive, his disease. There is no
‘subject’ in a narrow case history; modern case histories allude to the subject in a cursory phrase (‘a
trisomic albino female of 21’), which could as well apply to a rat as a human being. To restore the
human subject at the centre—the suffering, afflicted, fighting, human subject—we must deepen a case
history to a narrative or tale; only then do we have a ‘who’ as well as a ‘what’, a real person, a patient, in
relation to disease—in relation to the physical.
The patient’s essential being is very relevant in the higher reaches of neurology, and in psychology;
for here the patient’s personhood is essentially involved, and the study of disease and of identity cannot
be disjoined. Such disorders, and their depiction and study, indeed entail a new discipline, which we
may call the ‘neurology of identity’, for it deals with the neural foundations of the self, the age-old
problem of mind and brain. It is possible that there must, of necessity, be a gulf, a gulf of category,
between the psychical and the physical; but studies and stories pertaining simultaneously and
inseparably to both—and it is these which especially fascinate me, and which (on the whole) I present
here—may nonetheless serve to bring them nearer, to bring us to the very intersection of mechanism and
life, to the relation of physiological processes to biography.
The tradition of richly human clinical tales reached a high point in the nineteenth century, and then
declined, with the advent of an impersonal neurological science. Luria wrote: ‘The power to describe,
which was so common to the great nineteenth-century neurologists and psychiatrists, is almost gone
now. ... It must be revived.’ His own late works, such as The Mind of a Mnemonist and The Man with a
Shattered World, are attempts to revive this lost tradition. Thus the case-histories in this book hark back
to an ancient tradition: to the nineteenth-century tradition of which Luria speaks; to the tradition of the
first medical historian, Hippocrates; and to that universal and prehistorical tradition by which patients
have always told their stories to doctors.
Classical fables have archetypal figures—heroes, victims, martyrs, warriors. Neurological patients
are all of these—and in the strange tales told here they are also something more. How, in these mythical
or metaphorical terms, shall we categorize the ‘lost Mariner’, or the other strange figures in this book?
We may say they are travelers to unimaginable lands—lands of which otherwise we should have no idea
or conception. This is why their lives and journeys seem to me to have a quality of the fabulous, why I
have used Osier’s Arabian Nights image as an epigraph, and why I feel compelled to speak of tales and
fables as well as cases. The scientific and the romantic in such realms cry out to come together—Luria
liked to speak here of ‘romantic science’. They come together at the intersection of fact and fable, the
intersection which characterizes (as it did in my book Awakenings) the lives of the patients here
narrated.
But what facts! What fables! To what shall we compare them? We may not have any existing
models, metaphors or myths. Has the time perhaps come for new symbols, new myths?
Eight of the chapters in this book have already been published: ‘The Lost Mariner’, ‘Hands’, ‘The
Twins’, and ‘The Autist Artist’ in the New York Review of Books (1984 and 1985), and ‘Witty Ticcy
Ray’, ‘The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat’, and ‘Reminiscence’ in the London Review of Books
(1981, 1983, 1984)— where the briefer version of the last was called ‘Musical Ears’. ‘On the Level’ was
published in The Sciences (1985). A very early account of one of my patients—the ‘original’ of Rose R.
in Awakenings and of Harold Pinter’s Deborah in A Kind of Alaska, inspired by that book—is to be
found in ‘Incontinent Nostalgia’ (originally published as ‘Incontinent Nostalgia Induced by L-Dopa’ in
the Lancet of Spring 1970). Of my four ‘Phantoms’, the first two were published as ‘clinical curios’ in
the British Medical journal (1984). Two short pieces are taken from previous books: ‘The Man Who
Fell out of Bed’ is excerpted from A Leg to Stand On, and ‘The Visions of Hildegard’ from Migraine.
The remaining twelve pieces are unpublished and entirely new, and were all written during the autumn
and winter of 1984.
I owe a very special debt to my editors: first to Robert Silvers of the New York Review of Books and
Mary-Kay Wilmers of the London Review of Books; then to Kate Edgar, Jim Silberman of Summit
Rooks in New York, and Colin Haycraft of Duckworth’s in London, who between them did so much to
shape the final book.
Among my fellow neurologists I must express special gratitude to the late Dr James Purdon Martin,
to whom I showed videotapes of ‘Christina’ and ‘Mr. MacGregor’ and with whom I discussed these
patients fully—’The Disembodied Lady’ and ‘On the Level’ express this indebtedness; to Dr Michael
Kremer, my former ‘chief in London, who in response to A Leg to Stand On (1984) described a very
similar case of his own—these are bracketed together now in ‘The Man Who Fell out of Bed’; to Dr
Donald Macrae, whose extraordinary case of visual agnosia, almost comically similar to my own, was
only discovered, by accident, two years after I had written my own piece—it is excerpted in a postscript
to ‘The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat’; and, most especially, to my close friend and colleague,
Dr Isabelle Rapin, in New York, who discussed many cases with me; she introduced me to Christina
(the ‘disembodied lady’), and had known Jose, the ‘autist artist’, for many years when he was a child.
I wish to acknowledge the selfless help and generosity of the patients (and, in some cases, the
relatives of the patients) whose tales I tell here—who, knowing (as they often did) that they themselves
might not be able to be helped directly, yet permitted, even encouraged, me to write of their lives, in the
hope that others might learn and understand, and, one day, perhaps be able to cure. As in Awakenings,
names and some circumstantial details have been changed for reasons of personal and professional
confidence, but my aim has been to preserve the essential ‘feeling’ of their lives.
Finally, I wish to express my gratitude—more than gratitude— to my own mentor and physician, to
whom I dedicate this book.
To talk of diseases is a sort of Arabian Nights entertainment.
—William Osler
The physician is concerned [unlike the naturalist] ... with a single organism, the human subject, striving
to preserve its identity in adverse circumstances.
—Ivy McKenzie
PART ONE LOSSES
Introduction
Neurology’s favorite word is ‘deficit’, denoting an impairment or incapacity of neurological function:
loss of speech, loss of language, loss of memory, loss of vision, loss of dexterity, loss of identity and
myriad other lacks and losses of specific functions (or faculties). For all of these dysfunctions (another
favorite term), we have privative words of every sort—Aphonia, Aphemia, Aphasia, Alexia, Apraxia,
Agnosia, Amnesia, Ataxia—a word for every specific neural or mental function of which patients,
through disease, or injury, or failure to develop, may find themselves partly or wholly deprived.
The scientific study of the relationship between brain and mind began in 1861, when Broca, in
France, found that specific difficulties in the expressive use of speech, aphasia, consistently followed
damage to a particular portion of the left hemisphere of the brain. This opened the way to a cerebral
neurology, which made it possible, over the decades, to ‘map’ the human brain, ascribing specific
powers—linguistic, intellectual, perceptual, etc.—to equally specific ‘centers’ in the brain. Toward the
end of the century it became evident to more acute observers—above all to Freud, in his book
Aphasia—that this sort of mapping was too simple, that all mental performances had an intricate internal
structure, and must have an equally complex physiological basis. Freud felt this, especially, in regard to
certain disorders of recognition and perception, for which he coined the term ‘agnosia’. All adequate
understanding of aphasia or agnosia would, he believed, require a new, more sophisticated science.
The new science of brain/mind which Freud envisaged came into being in the Second World War, in
Russia, as the joint creation of A. R. Luria (and his father, R. A. Luria), Leontev, Anokhin, Bernstein
and others, and was called by them ‘neuropsychology.’ The development of this immensely fruitful
science was the lifework of A. R. Luria, and considering its revolutionary importance it was somewhat
slow in reaching the West. It was set out, systematically, in a monumental book, Higher Cortical
Functions in Man (Eng. tr. 1966) and, in a wholly different way, in a biography or ‘pathography’—The
Man with a Shattered World (Eng. tr. 1972). Although these books were almost perfect in their way,
there was a whole realm which Luria had not touched. Higher Cortical Functions in Man treated only
those functions which appertained to the left hemisphere of the brain; similarly, Zazetsky, subject of The
Man with a Shattered World, had a huge lesion in the left hemisphere—the right was intact. Indeed, the
entire history of neurology and neuropsychology can be seen as a history of the investigation of the left
hemisphere.
One important reason for the neglect of the right, or ‘minor’, hemisphere, as it has always been
called, is that while it is easy to demonstrate the effects of variously located lesions on the left side, the
corresponding syndromes of the right hemisphere are much less distinct. It was presumed, usually
contemptuously, to be more ‘primitive’ than the left, the latter being seen as the unique flower of human
evolution. And in a sense this is correct: the left hemisphere is more sophisticated and specialized, a
very late outgrowth of the primate, and especially the hominid, brain. On the other hand, it is the right
hemisphere which controls the crucial powers of recognizing reality which every living creature must
have in order to survive. The left hemisphere, like a computer tacked onto the basic creatural brain, is
designed for programs and schematics; and classical neurology was more concerned with schematics
than with reality, so that when, at last, some of the right hemisphere syndromes emerged, they were
considered bizarre.
There had been attempts in the past—for example, by Anton in the 1890s and Potzl in 1928—to
explore right hemisphere syndromes, but these attempts themselves had been bizarrely ignored.
In The Working Brain, one of his last books, Luria devoted a short but tantalizing section to right
hemisphere syndromes, ending:
These still completely unstudied defects lead us to one of the most fundamental problems—to the
role of the right hemisphere in direct consciousness.... The study of this highly important field has been
so far neglected. ... It will receive a detailed analysis in a special series of papers ... in preparation for
publication.
Luria did, finally, write some of these papers, in the last months of his life, when mortally ill. He
never saw their publication, nor were they published in Russia. He sent them to R. L. Gregory in
England, and they will appear in Gregory’s forthcoming Oxford Companion to the Mind.
Inner difficulties and outer difficulties match each other here. It is not only difficult, it is impossible,
for patients with certain right hemisphere syndromes to know their own problems—a peculiar and
specific ‘anosagnosia’, as Babinski called it. And it is singularly difficult, for even the most sensitive
observer, to picture the inner state, the ‘situation’, of such patients, for this is almost unimaginably
remote from anything he himself has ever known. Left hemisphere syndromes, by contrast, are relatively
easily imagined. Although right hemisphere syndromes are as common as left hemisphere syndromes—
why should they not be?—we will find a thousand descriptions of left hemisphere syndromes in the
neurological and neuropsychological literature for every description of a right hemisphere syndrome. It
is as if such syndromes were somehow alien to the whole temper of neurology. And yet, as Luria says,
they are of the most fundamental importance. So much so that they may demand a new sort of
neurology, a ‘personalistic’, or (as Luria liked to call it) a ‘romantic’, science; for the physical
foundations of the persona, the self, are here revealed for our study. Luria thought a science of this kind
would be best introduced by a story—a detailed case-history of a man with a profound right hemisphere
disturbance, a case-history which would at once be the complement and opposite of ‘the man with a
shattered world.’ In one of his last letters to me he wrote: ‘Publish such histories, even if they are just
sketches. It is a realm of great wonder.’ I must confess to being especially intrigued by these disorders,
for they open realms, or promise realms, scarcely imagined before, pointing to an open and more
spacious neurology and psychology, excitingly different from the rather rigid and mechanical neurology
of the past.
It is, then, less deficits, in the traditional sense, which have engaged my interest than neurological
disorders affecting the self. Such disorders may be of many kinds—and may arise from excesses, no less
than impairments, of function—and it seems reasonable to consider these two categories separately. But
it must be said from the outset that a disease is never a mere loss or excess— that there is always a
reaction, on the part of the affected organism or individual, to restore, to replace, to compensate for and
to preserve its identity, however strange the means may be: and to study or influence these means, no
less than the primary insult to the nervous system, is an essential part of our role as physicians. This was
powerfully stated by Ivy McKenzie:
For what is it that constitutes a ‘disease entity’ or a ‘new disease’? The physician is concerned not,
like the naturalist, with a wide range of different organisms theoretically adapted in an average way to
an average environment, but with a single organism, the human subject, striving to preserve its identity
in adverse circumstances.
This dynamic, this ‘striving to preserve identity’, however strange the means or effects of such
striving, was recognized in psychiatry long ago—and, like so much else, is especially associated with
the work of Freud. Thus, the delusions of paranoia were seen by him not as primary but as attempts
(however misguided) at restitution, at reconstructing a world reduced by complete chaos. In precisely
the same way, Ivy McKenzie wrote:
The pathological physiology of the Parkinsonian syndrome is the study of an organized chaos, a
chaos induced in the first instance by destruction of important integrations, and reorganized on an
unstable basis in the process of rehabilitation.
As Awakenings was the study of ‘an organized chaos’ produced by a single if multiform disease, so
what now follows is a series of similar studies of the organized chaoses produced by a great variety of
diseases.
In this first section, ‘Losses’, the most important case, to my mind, is that of a special form of visual
agnosia: ‘The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat’. I believe it to be of fundamental importance. Such
cases constitute a radical challenge to one of the most entrenched axioms or assumptions of classical
neurology—in particular, the notion that brain damage, any brain damage, reduces or removes the
‘abstract and categorical attitude’ (in Kurt Goldstein’s term), reducing the individual to the emotional
and concrete. (A very similar thesis was made by Hughlings Jackson in the 1860s.) Here, in the case of
Dr P., we see the very opposite of this—a man who has (albeit only in the sphere of the visual) wholly
lost the emotional, the concrete, the personal, the ‘real’ ... and been reduced, as it were, to the abstract
and the categorical, with consequences of a particularly preposterous kind. What would Hughlings
Jackson and Goldstein have said of this? I have often in imagination, asked them to examine Dr P., and
then said, ‘Gentlemen! What do you say now?’
1 The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat
Dr P. was a musician of distinction, well-known for many years as a singer, and then, at the local
School of Music, as a teacher. It was here, in relation to his students, that certain strange problems were
first observed. Sometimes a student would present himself, and Dr P. would not recognize him; or,
specifically, would not recognize his face. The moment the student spoke, he would be recognized by
his voice. Such incidents multiplied, causing embarrassment, perplexity, fear—and, sometimes, comedy.
For not only did Dr P. increasingly fail to see faces, but he saw faces when there were no faces to see:
genially, Magoo-like, when in the street he might pat the heads of water hydrants and parking meters,
taking these to be the heads of children; he would amiably address carved knobs on the furniture and be
astounded when they did not reply. At first these odd mistakes were laughed off as jokes, not least by Dr
P. himself. Had he not always had a quirky sense of humor and been given to Zen-like paradoxes and
jests? His musical powers were as dazzling as ever; he did not feel ill—he had never felt better; and the
mistakes were so ludicrous—and so ingenious—that they could hardly be serious or betoken anything
serious. The notion of there being ‘something the matter’ did not emerge until some three years later,
when diabetes developed. Well aware that diabetes could affect his eyes, Dr P. consulted an
ophthalmologist, who took a careful history and examined his eyes closely. ‘There’s nothing the matter
with your eyes,’ the doctor concluded. ‘But there is trouble with the visual parts of your brain.
You don’t need my help, you must see a neurologist.’ And so, as a result of this referral, Dr P. came
to me.
It was obvious within a few seconds of meeting him that there was no trace of dementia in the
ordinary sense. He was a man of great cultivation and charm who talked well and fluently, with
imagination and humor. I couldn’t think why he had been referred to our clinic.
And yet there was something a bit odd. He faced me as he spoke, was oriented towards me, and yet
there was something the matter—it was difficult to formulate. He faced me with his ears, I came to
think, but not with his eyes. These, instead of looking, gazing, at me, ‘taking me in’, in the normal way,
made sudden strange fixations—on my nose, on my right ear, down to my chin, up to my right eye—as
if noting (even studying) these individual features, but not seeing my whole face, its changing
expressions, ‘me’, as a whole. I am not sure that I fully realized this at the time—there was just a teasing
strangeness, some failure in the normal interplay of gaze and expression. He saw me, he scanned me,
and yet ...
‘What seems to be the matter?’ I asked him at length.
‘Nothing that I know of,’ he replied with a smile, ‘but people seem to think there’s something wrong
with my eyes.’
‘But you don’t recognize any visual problems?’
‘No, not directly, but I occasionally make mistakes.’
I left the room briefly to talk to his wife. When I came back, Dr P. was sitting placidly by the
window, attentive, listening rather than looking out. ‘Traffic,’ he said, ‘street sounds, distant trains—
they make a sort of symphony, do they not? You know Honegger’s Pacific 234?’
What a lovely man, I thought to myself. How can there be anything seriously the matter? Would he
permit me to examine him?
‘Yes, of course, Dr Sacks.’
I stilled my disquiet, his perhaps, too, in the soothing routine of a neurological exam—muscle
strength, coordination, reflexes, tone. ... It was while examining his reflexes—a trifle abnormal on the
left side—that the first bizarre experience occurred. I had taken off his left shoe and scratched the sole of
his foot with a key—a frivolous-seeming but essential test of a reflex—and then, excusing myself to
screw my ophthalmoscope together, left him to put on the shoe himself. To my surprise, a minute later,
he had not done this.
‘Can I help?’ I asked.
‘Help what? Help whom?’
‘Help you put on your shoe.’
‘Ach,’ he said, ‘I had forgotten the shoe,’ adding, sotto voce, ‘The shoe? The shoe?’ He seemed
baffled.
‘Your shoe,’ I repeated. ‘Perhaps you’d put it on.’
He continued to look downwards, though not at the shoe, with an intense but misplaced
concentration. Finally his gaze settled on his foot: ‘That is my shoe, yes?’
Did I mis-hear? Did he mis-see?
‘My eyes,’ he explained, and put a hand to his foot. ‘This is my shoe, no?’
‘No, it is not. That is your foot. There is your shoe.’
‘Ah! I thought that was my foot.’
Was he joking? Was he mad? Was he blind? If this was one of his ‘strange mistakes’, it was the
strangest mistake I had ever come across.
I helped him on with his shoe (his foot), to avoid further complication. Dr P. himself seemed
untroubled, indifferent, maybe amused. I resumed my examination. His visual acuity was good: he had
no difficulty seeing a pin on the floor, though sometimes he missed it if it was placed to his left.
He saw all right, but what did he see? I opened out a copy of the National Geographic Magazine and
asked him to describe some pictures in it.
His responses here were very curious. His eyes would dart from one thing to another, picking up tiny
features, individual features, as they had done with my face. A striking brightness, a color, a shape
would arrest his attention and elicit comment—but in no case did he get the scene-as-a-whole. He failed
to see the whole, seeing only details, which he spotted like blips on a radar screen. He never entered into
relation with the picture as a whole—never faced, so to speak, its physiognomy. He had no sense
whatever of a landscape or scene.
I showed him the cover, an unbroken expanse of Sahara dunes.
‘What do you see here?’ I asked.
‘I see a river,’ he said. ‘And a little guest-house with its terrace on the water. People are dining out on
the terrace. I see colored parasols here and there.’ He was looking, if it was ‘looking’, right off the cover
into midair and confabulating nonexistent features, as if the absence of features in the actual picture had
driven him to imagine the river and the terrace and the colored parasols.
I must have looked aghast, but he seemed to think he had done rather well. There was a hint of a
smile on his face. He also appeared to have decided that the examination was over and started to look
around for his hat. He reached out his hand and took hold of his wife’s head, tried to lift it off, to put it
on. He had apparently mistaken his wife for a hat! His wife looked as if she was used to such things.
I could make no sense of what had occurred in terms of conventional neurology (or
neuropsychology). In some ways he seemed perfectly preserved, and in others absolutely,
incomprehensibly devastated. How could he, on the one hand, mistake his wife for a hat and, on the
other, function, as apparently he still did, as a teacher at the Music School?
I had to think, to see him again—and to see him in his own familiar habitat, at home.
A few days later I called on Dr P. and his wife at home, with the score of the Dichterliebe in my
briefcase (I knew he liked Schumann), and a variety of odd objects for the testing of perception. Mrs P.
showed me into a lofty apartment, which recalled fin-de-siecle Berlin. A magnificent old Bosendorfer
stood in state in the centre of the room, and all around it were music stands, instruments, scores.... There
were books, there were paintings, but the music was central. Dr P. came in, a little bowed, and,
distracted, advanced with outstretched hand to the grandfather clock, but, hearing my voice, corrected
himself, and shook hands with me. We exchanged greetings and chatted a little of current concerts and
performances. Diffidently, I asked him if he would sing.
The Dichterliebe!’ he exclaimed. ‘But I can no longer read music. You will play them, yes?’
I said I would try. On that wonderful old piano even my playing sounded right, and Dr P. was an
aged but infinitely mellow Fischer-Dieskau, combining a perfect ear and voice with the most incisive
musical intelligence. It was clear that the Music School was not keeping him on out of charity.
Dr P. ‘s temporal lobes were obviously intact: he had a wonderful musical cortex. What, I wondered,
was going on in his parietal and occipital lobes, especially in those areas where visual processing
occurred? I carry the Platonic solids in my neurological kit and decided to start with these.
‘What is this?’ I asked, drawing out the first one.
‘A cube, of course.’
‘Now this?’ I asked, brandishing another.
He asked if he might examine it, which he did swiftly and systematically: ‘A dodecahedron, of
course. And don’t bother with the others—I’ll get the icosahedron, too.’
Abstract shapes clearly presented no problems. What about faces? I took out a pack of cards. All of
these he identified instantly, including the jacks, queens, kings, and the joker. But these, after all, are
stylized designs, and it was impossible to tell whether he saw faces or merely patterns. I decided I would
show him a volume of cartoons which I had in my briefcase. Here, again, for the most part, he did well.
Churchill’s cigar, Schnozzle’s nose: as soon as he had picked out a key feature he could identify the
face. But cartoons, again, are formal and schematic. It remained to be seen how he would do with real
faces, realistically represented.
I turned on the television, keeping the sound off, and found an early Bette Davis film. A love scene
was in progress. Dr P. failed to identify the actress—but this could have been because she had never
entered his world. What was more striking was that he failed to identify the expressions on her face or
her partner’s, though in the course of a single torrid scene these passed from sultry yearning through
passion, surprise, disgust, and fury to a melting reconciliation. Dr P. could make nothing of any of this.
He was very unclear as to what was going on, or who was who or even what sex they were. His
comments on the scene were positively Martian.
It was just possible that some of his difficulties were associated with the unreality of a celluloid,
Hollywood world; and it occurred to me that he might be more successful in identifying faces from his
own life. On the walls of the apartment there were photographs of his family, his colleagues, his pupils,
himself. I gathered a pile of these together and, with some misgivings, presented them to him. What had
been funny, or farcical, in relation to the movie, was tragic in relation to real life. By and large, he
recognized nobody: neither his family, nor his colleagues, nor his pupils, nor himself. He recognized a
portrait of Einstein because he picked up the characteristic hair and moustache; and the same thing
happened with one or two other people. ‘Ach, Paul!’ he said, when shown a portrait of his brother. ‘That
square jaw, those big teeth— I would know Paul anywhere!’ But was it Paul he recognized, or one or
two of his features, on the basis of which he could make a reasonable guess as to the subject’s identity?
In the absence of obvious ‘markers’, he was utterly lost. But it was not merely the cognition, the gnosis,
at fault; there was something radically wrong with the whole way he proceeded. For he approached
these faces— even of those near and dear—as if they were abstract puzzles or tests. He did not relate to
them, he did not behold. No face was familiar to him, seen as a ‘thou’, being just identified as a set of
features, an ‘it’. Thus, there was formal, but no trace of personal, gnosis. And with this went his
indifference, or blindness, to expression. A face, to us, is a person looking out—we see, as it were, the
person through his persona, his face. But for Dr P. there was no persona in this sense—no outward
persona, and no person within.
I had stopped at a florist on my way to his apartment and bought myself an extravagant red rose for
my buttonhole. Now I removed this and handed it to him. He took it like a botanist or morphologist
given a specimen, not like a person given a flower.
About six inches in length,’ he commented. ‘A convoluted red form with a linear green attachment.’
‘Yes,’ I said encouragingly, ‘and what do you think it is, Dr P.?’
‘Not easy to say.’ He seemed perplexed. ‘It lacks the simple symmetry of the Platonic solids,
although it may have a higher symmetry of its own. ... I think this could be an inflorescence or flower.’
‘Could be?’ I queried.
‘Could be,’ he confirmed.
‘Smell it,’ I suggested, and he again looked somewhat puzzled, as if I had asked him to smell a
higher symmetry. But he complied courteously, and took it to his nose. Now, suddenly, he came to life.
‘Beautiful!’ he exclaimed. ‘An early rose. What a heavenly smell!’ He started to hum ‘Die Rose, die
Lillie ...’ Reality, it seemed, might be conveyed by smell, not by sight.
I tried one final test. It was still a cold day, in early spring, and I had thrown my coat and gloves on
the sofa.
‘What is this?’ I asked, holding up a glove.
‘May I examine it?’ he asked, and, taking it from me, he proceeded to examine it as he had examined
the geometrical shapes.
‘A continuous surface,’ he announced at last, ‘infolded on itself. It appears to have’—he hesitated—
’five outpouchings, if this is the word.’
‘Yes,’ I said cautiously. You have given me a description. Now tell me what it is.’
‘A container of some sort?’
Yes,’ I said, ‘and what would it contain?’
‘It would contain its contents!’ said Dr P., with a laugh. ‘There are many possibilities. It could be a
change purse, for example, for coins of five sizes. It could ...’
I interrupted the barmy flow. ‘Does it not look familiar? Do you think it might contain, might fit, a
part of your body?’
No light of recognition dawned on his face.(Later, by accident, he got it on, and exclaimed, ‘My God,
it’s a glove!’ This was reminiscent of Kurt Goldstein’s patient ‘Lanuti’, who could only recognize
objects by trying to use them in action.)
No child would have the power to see and speak of ‘a continuous surface ... infolded on itself,’ but
any child, any infant, would immediately know a glove as a glove, see it as familiar, as going with a
hand. Dr P. didn’t. He saw nothing as familiar. Visually, he was lost in a world of lifeless abstractions.
Indeed, he did not have a real visual world, as he did not have a real visual self. He could speak about
things, but did not see them face-to-face. Hughlings Jackson, discussing patients with aphasia and left
hemisphere lesions, says they have lost ‘abstract’ and ‘propositional’ thought—and compares them with
dogs (or, rather, he compares dogs to patients with aphasia). Dr P., on the other hand, functioned
precisely as a machine functions. It wasn’t merely that he displayed the same indifference to the visual
world as a computer but—even more strikingly—he construed the world as a computer construes it, by
means of key features and schematic relationships. The scheme might be identified—in an ‘identi-kit’
way—without the reality being grasped at all.
The testing I had done so far told me nothing about Dr P.’s inner world. Was it possible that his
visual memory and imagination were still intact? I asked him to imagine entering one of our local
squares from the north side, to walk through it, in imagination or in memory, and tell me the buildings
he might pass as he walked. He listed the buildings on his right side, but none of those on his left. I then
asked him to imagine entering the square from the south. Again he mentioned only those buildings that
were on the right side, although these were the very buildings he had omitted before. Those he had
‘seen’ internally before were not mentioned now; presumably, they were no longer ‘seen’. It was evident
that his difficulties with leftness, his visual field deficits, were as much internal as external, bisecting his
visual memory and imagination.
What, at a higher level, of his internal visualization? Thinking of the almost hallucinatory intensity
with which Tolstoy visualizes and animates his characters, I questioned Dr P. about Anna Karenina. He
could remember incidents without difficulty, had an undiminished grasp of the plot, but completely
omitted visual characteristics, visual narrative, and scenes. He remembered the words of the characters
but not their faces; and though, when asked, he could quote, with his remarkable and almost verbatim
memory, the original visual descriptions, these were, it became apparent, quite empty for him and lacked
sensorial, imaginal, or emotional reality. Thus, there was an internal agnosia as well (I have often
wondered about Helen Keller’s visual descriptions, whether these, for all their eloquence, are somehow
empty as well? Or whether, by the transference of images from the tactile to the visual, or, yet more
extraordinarily, from the verbal and the metaphorical to the sensorial and the visual, she did achieve a
power of visual imagery, even though her visual cortex had never been stimulated, directly, by the eyes?
But in Dr P.’s case it is precisely the cortex that was damaged, the organic prerequisite of all pictorial
imagery. Interestingly and typically he no longer dreamed pictorally—the ‘message’ of the dream being
conveyed in nonvisual terms.)
But this was only the case, it became clear, with certain sorts of visualization. The visualization of faces
and scenes, of visual narrative and drama—this was profoundly impaired, almost absent. But the
visualization of schemata was preserved, perhaps enhanced. Thus, when I engaged him in a game of
mental chess, he had no difficulty visualizing the chessboard or the moves— indeed, no difficulty in
beating me soundly.
Luria said of Zazetsky that he had entirely lost his capacity to play games but that his ‘vivid
imagination’ was unimpaired. Zazetsky and Dr P. lived in worlds which were mirror images of each
other. But the saddest difference between them was that Zazetsky, as Luria said, ‘fought to regain his
lost faculties with the indomitable tenacity of the damned,’ whereas Dr P. was not fighting, did not know
what was lost, did not indeed know that anything was lost. But who was more tragic, or who was more
damned— the man who knew it, or the man who did not?
When the examination was over, Mrs P. called us to the table, where there was coffee and a delicious
spread of little cakes. Hungrily, hummingly, Dr P. started on the cakes. Swiftly, fluently, unthinkingly,
melodiously, he pulled the plates towards him and took this and that in a great gurgling stream, an edible
song of food, until, suddenly, there came an interruption: a loud, peremptory rat-tat-tat at the door.
Startled, taken aback, arrested by the interruption, Dr P. stopped eating and sat frozen, motionless, at the
table, with an indifferent, blind bewilderment on his face. He saw, but no longer saw, the table; no
longer perceived it as a table laden with cakes. His wife poured him some coffee: the smell titillated his
nose and brought him back to reality. The melody of eating resumed.
How does he do anything? I wondered to myself. What happens when he’s dressing, goes to the
lavatory, has a bath? I followed his wife into the kitchen and asked her how, for instance, he managed to
dress himself. ‘It’s just like the eating,’ she explained. ‘I put his usual clothes out, in all the usual places,
and he dresses without difficulty, singing to himself. He does everything singing to himself. But if he is
interrupted and loses the thread, he comes to a complete stop, doesn’t know his clothes—or his own
body. He sings all the time—eating songs, dressing songs, bathing songs, everything. He can’t do
anything unless he makes it a song.’
While we were talking my attention was caught by the pictures on the walls.
‘Yes,’ Mrs P. said, ‘he was a gifted painter as well as a singer. The School exhibited his pictures
every year.’
I strolled past them curiously—they were in chronological order. All his earlier work was naturalistic
and realistic, with vivid mood and atmosphere, but finely detailed and concrete. Then, years later, they
became less vivid, less concrete, less realistic and naturalistic, but far more abstract, even geometrical
and cubist. Finally, in the last paintings, the canvasses became nonsense, or nonsense to me—mere
chaotic lines and blotches of paint. I commented on this to Mrs P.
‘Ach, you doctors, you’re such Philistines!’ she exclaimed. ‘Can you not see artistic development—
how he renounced the realism of his earlier years, and advanced into abstract, nonrepresentational art?’
‘No, that’s not it,’ I said to myself (but forbore to say it to poor Mrs P.). He had indeed moved from
realism to nonrepresentation to the abstract, yet this was not the artist, but the pathology, advancing—
advancing towards a profound visual agnosia, in which all powers of representation and imagery, all
sense of the concrete, all sense of reality, were being destroyed. This wall of paintings was a tragic
pathological exhibit, which belonged to neurology, not art.
And yet, I wondered, was she not partly right? For there is often a struggle, and sometimes, even
more interestingly, a collusion between the powers of pathology and creation. Perhaps, in his cubist
period, there might have been both artistic and pathological development, colluding to engender an
original form; for as he lost the concrete, so he might have gained in the abstract, developing a greater
sensitivity to all the structural elements of line, boundary, contour—an almost Picasso-like power to see,
and equally depict, those abstract organizations embedded in, and normally lost in, the concrete....
Though in the final pictures, I feared, there was only chaos and agnosia.
We returned to the great music room, with the Bosendorfer in the centre, and Dr P. humming the last
torte.
‘Well, Dr Sacks,’ he said to me. ‘You find me an interesting case, I perceive. Can you tell me what
you find wrong, make recommendations?’
‘I can’t tell you what I find wrong,’ I replied, ‘but I’ll say what I find right. You are a wonderful
musician, and music is your life. What I would prescribe, in a case such as yours, is a life which consists
entirely of music. Music has been the centre, now make it the whole, of your life.’
This was four years ago—I never saw him again, but I often wondered about how he apprehended the
world, given his strange loss of image, visuality, and the perfect preservation of a great musicality. I
think that music, for him, had taken the place of image. He had no body-image, he had body-music: this
is why he could move and act as fluently as he did, but came to a total confused stop if the ‘inner music’
stopped. And equally with the outside, the world ... (Thus, as I learned later from his wife, though he
could not recognize his students if they sat still, if they were merely ‘images’, he might suddenly
recognize them if they moved. ‘That’s Karl,’ he would cry. ‘I know his movements, his body-music’)
In The World as Representation and Will, Schopenhauer speaks of music as ‘pure will’. How
fascinated he would have been by Dr P., a man who had wholly lost the world as representation, but
wholly preserved it as music or will.
And this, mercifully, held to the end—for despite the gradual advance of his disease (a massive
tumor or degenerative process in the visual parts of his brain) Dr P. lived and taught music to the last
days of his life.
Postscript
How should one interpret Dr P.’s peculiar inability to interpret, to judge, a glove as a glove?
Manifestly, here, he could not make a cognitive judgment, though he was prolific in the production of
cognitive hypotheses. A judgment is intuitive, personal, comprehensive, and concrete—we ‘see’ how
things stand, in relation to one another and oneself. It was precisely this setting, this relating, that Dr P.
lacked (though his judging, in all other spheres, was prompt and normal). Was this due to lack of visual
information, or faulty processing of visual information? (This would be the explanation given by a
classical, schematic neurology.) Or was there something amiss in Dr P.’s attitude, so that he could not
relate what he saw to himself?
These explanations, or modes of explanation, are not mutually exclusive—being in different modes
they could coexist and both be true. And this is acknowledged, implicitly or explicitly, in classical
neurology: implicitly, by Macrae, when he finds the explanation of defective schemata, or defective
visual processing and integration, inadequate; explicitly, by Goldstein, when he speaks of ‘abstract
attitude’. But abstract attitude, which allows ‘categorization’, also misses the mark with Dr P.—and,
perhaps, with the concept of ‘judgment’ in general. For Dr P. had abstract attitude— indeed, nothing
else. And it was precisely this, his absurd abstractness of attitude—absurd because unleavened with
anything else—which rendered him incapable of perceiving identity, or particulars, rendered him
incapable of judgment.
Neurology and psychology, curiously, though they talk of everything else, almost never talk of
‘judgment’—and yet it is precisely the downfall of judgment (whether in specific realms, as with Dr P.,
or more generally, as in patients with Korsakov’s or frontal-lobe syndromes—see below, Chapters
Twelve and Thirteen) which constitutes the essence of so many neuropsychological disorders.
Judgment and identity may be casualties—but neuropsychology never speaks of them.
And yet, whether in a philosophic sense (Kant’s sense), or an empirical and evolutionary sense,
judgment is the most important faculty we have. An animal, or a man, may get on very well without
‘abstract attitude’ but will speedily perish if deprived of judgment. Judgment must be the first faculty of
higher life or mind—yet it is ignored, or misinterpreted, by classical (computational) neurology. And if
we wonder how such an absurdity can arise, we find it in the assumptions, or the evolution, of neurology
itself. For classical neurology (like classical physics) has always been mechanical—from Hughlings
Jackson’s mechanical analogies to the computer analogies of today.
Of course, the brain is a machine and a computer—everything in classical neurology is correct. But
our mental processes, which constitute our being and life, are not just abstract and mechanical, but
personal, as well—and, as such, involve not just classifying and categorizing, but continual judging and
feeling also. If this is missing, we become computer-like, as Dr P. was. And, by the same token, if we
delete feeling and judging, the personal, from the cognitive sciences, we reduce them to something as
defective as Dr P.—and we reduce our apprehension of the concrete and real.
By a sort of comic and awful analogy, our current cognitive neurology and psychology resemble
nothing so much as poor Dr P.! We need the concrete and real, as he did; and we fail to see this, as he
failed to see it. Our cognitive sciences are themselves suffering from an agnosia essentially similar to Dr
P.’s. Dr P. may therefore serve as a warning and parable—of what happens to a science which eschews
the judgmental, the particular, the personal, and becomes entirely abstract and computational.
It was always a matter of great regret to me that, owing to circumstances beyond my control, I was
not able to follow his case further, either in the sort of observations and investigations described, or in
ascertaining the actual disease pathology.
One always fears that a case is ‘unique’, especially if it has such extraordinary features as those of Dr
P. It was, therefore, with a sense of great interest and delight, not unmixed with relief, that I found, quite
by chance—looking through the periodical Brain for 1956—a detailed description of an almost
comically similar case, similar (indeed identical) neuropsychologically and phenomenologically, though
the underlying pathology (an acute head injury) and all personal circumstances were wholly different.
The authors speak of their case as ‘unique in the documented history of this disorder’—and evidently
experienced, as I did, amazement at their own findings. The interested reader is referred to the original
paper, Macrae and Trolle (1956), of which I here subjoin a brief paraphrase, with quotations from the
original. Only since the completion of this book have I found that there is, in fact, a rather extensive
literature on visual agnosia in general, and prosopagnosia in particular. In particular I had the great
pleasure recently of meeting Dr Andrew Kertesz, who has himself published some extremely detailed
studies of patients with such agnosias (see, for example, his paper on visual agnosia, Kertesz 1979). Dr
Kertesz mentioned to me a case known to him of a farmer who had developed prosopagnosia and in
consequence could no longer distinguish (the faces of) his cows, and of another such patient, an
attendant in a Natural History Museum, who mistook his own reflection for the diorama of an ape. As
with Dr P., and as with Macrae and Trolle’s patient, it is especially the animate which is so absurdly
misperceived. The most important studies of such agnosias, and of visual processing in general, are now
being undertaken by A. R. and H. Damasio (see article in Mesulam [1985], pp. 259-288; or see p. 79
below).
Their patient was a young man of 32, who, following a severe automobile accident, with
unconsciousness for three weeks, ‘... complained, exclusively, of an inability to recognize faces, even
those of his wife and children’. Not a single face was ‘familiar’ to him, but there were three he could
identify; these were workmates: one with an eye-blinking tic, one with a large mole on his cheek, and a
third ‘because he was so tall and thin that no one else was like him’. Each of these, Macrae and Trolle
bring out, was ‘recognized solely by the single prominent feature mentioned’. In general (like Dr P.) he
recognized familiars only by their voices.
He had difficulty even recognizing himself in a mirror, as Macrae and Trolle describe in detail: ‘In
the early convalescent phase he frequently, especially when shaving, questioned whether the face gazing
at him was really his own, and even though he knew it could physically be none other, on several
occasions grimaced or stuck out his tongue “just to make sure.” By carefully studying his face in the
mirror he slowly began to recognize it, but “not in a flash” as in the past—he relied on the hair and facial
outline, and on two small moles on his left cheek.’
In general he could not recognize objects ‘at a glance’, but would have to seek out, and guess from,
one or two features— occasionally his guesses were absurdly wrong. In particular, the authors note,
there was difficulty with the animate.
On the other hand, simple schematic objects—scissors, watch, key, etc.—presented no difficulties.
Macrae and Trolle also note that: ‘His topographical memory was strange: the seeming paradox existed
that he could find his way from home to hospital and around the hospital, but yet could not name streets
en route [unlike Dr P., he also had some aphasia] or appear to visualize the topography.’
It was also evident that visual memories of people, even from long before the accident, were severely
impaired—there was memory of conduct, or perhaps a mannerism, but not of visual appearance or face.
Similarly, it appeared, when he was questioned closely, that he no longer had visual images in his
dreams. Thus, as with Dr P., it was not just visual perception, but visual imagination and memory, the
fundamental powers of visual representation, which were essentially damaged in this patient—at least
those powers insofar as they pertained to the personal, the familiar, the concrete.
A final, humorous point. Where Dr P. might mistake his wife for a hat, Macrae’s patient, also unable
to recognize his wife, needed her to identify herself by a visual marker, by ‘... a conspicuous article of
clothing, such as a large hat’.
2 The Lost Mariner
After writing and publishing this history I embarked with Dr Elkhonon Goldberg— a pupil of Luria
and editor of the original (Russian) edition of The Neuropsychology of Memory—on a close and
systematic neuropsychological study of this patient. Dr Goldberg has presented some of the preliminary
findings at conferences, and we hope in due course to publish a full account.
A deeply moving and extraordinary film about a patient with a profound amnesia (Prisoner of
Consciousness), made by Dr Jonathan Miller, has just been shown in England (September 1986). A film
has also been made (by Hilary Lawson) with a prosopagnosic patient (with many similarities to Dr P.).
Such films are crucial to assist the imagination: ‘What can be shown cannot be said.’
You have to begin to lose your memory, if only in bits and pieces, to realize that memory is what makes
our lives. Life without memory is no life at all ... Our memory is our coherence, our reason, our feeling,
even our action. Without it, we are nothing ... (I can only wait for the final amnesia, the one that can
erase an entire life, as it did my mother’s ...)
—Luis Bunuel
This moving and frightening segment in Bunuel’s recently translated memoirs raises fundamental
questions—clinical, practical, existential, philosophical: what sort of a life (if any), what sort of a world,
what sort of a self, can be preserved in a man who has lost the greater part of his memory and, with this,
his past, and his moorings in time?
It immediately made me think of a patient of mine in whom these questions are precisely
exemplified: charming, intelligent, memoryless Jimmie G., who was admitted to our Home for the Aged
near New York City early in 1975, with a cryptic transfer note saying, ‘Helpless, demented, confused
and disoriented.’
Jimmie was a fine-looking man, with a curly bush of grey hair, a healthy and handsome forty-nineyear-old. He was cheerful, friendly, and warm.
‘Hiya, Doc!’ he said. ‘Nice morning! Do I take this chair here?’ He was a genial soul, very ready to
talk and to answer any questions I asked him. He told me his name and birth date, and the name of the
little town in Connecticut where he was born. He described it in affectionate detail, even drew me a
map. He spoke of the houses where his family had lived—he remembered their phone numbers still. He
spoke of school and school days, the friends he’d had, and his special fondness for mathematics and
science. He talked with enthusiasm of his days in the navy—he was seventeen, had just graduated from
high school when he was drafted in 1943. With his good engineering mind he was a ‘natural’ for radio
and electronics, and after a crash course in Texas found himself assistant radio operator on a submarine.
He remembered the names of various submarines on which he had served, their missions, where they
were stationed, the names of his shipmates. He remembered Morse code, and was still fluent in Morse
tapping and touch-typing.
A full and interesting early life, remembered vividly, in detail, with affection. But there, for some
reason, his reminiscences stopped. He recalled, and almost relived, his war days and service, the end of
the war, and his thoughts for the future. He had come to love the navy, thought he might stay in it. But
with the GI Bill, and support, he felt he might do best to go to college. His older brother was in
accountancy school and engaged to a girl, a ‘real beauty’, from Oregon.
With recalling, reliving, Jimmie was full of animation; he did not seem to be speaking of the past but
of the present, and I was very struck by the change of tense in his recollections as he passed from his
school days to his days in the navy. He had been using the past tense, but now used the present—and (it
seemed to me) not just the formal or fictitious present tense of recall, but the actual present tense of
immediate experience.
A sudden, improbable suspicion seized me.
‘What year is this, Mr G.?’ I asked, concealing my perplexity under a casual manner.
‘Forty-five, man. What do you mean?’ He went on, ‘We’ve won the war, FDR’s dead, Truman’s at
the helm. There are great times ahead.’
‘And you, Jimmie, how old would you be?’
Oddly, uncertainly, he hesitated a moment, as if engaged in calculation.
‘Why, I guess I’m nineteen, Doc. I’ll be twenty next birthday.’
Looking at the grey-haired man before me, I had an impulse for which I have never forgiven
myself—it was, or would have been, the height of cruelty had there been any possibility of Jimmie’s
remembering it.
‘Here,’ I said, and thrust a mirror toward him. ‘Look in the mirror and tell me what you see. Is that a
nineteen-year-old looking out from the mirror?’
He suddenly turned ashen and gripped the sides of the chair. ‘Jesus Christ,’ he whispered. ‘Christ,
what’s going on? What’s happened to me? Is this a nightmare? Am I crazy? Is this a joke?’— and he
became frantic, panicked.
‘It’s okay, Jimmie,’ I said soothingly. ‘It’s just a mistake. Nothing to worry about. Hey!’ I took him
to the window. ‘Isn’t this a lovely spring day. See the kids there playing baseball?’ He regained his color
and started to smile, and I stole away, taking the hateful mirror with me.
Two minutes later I re-entered the room. Jimmie was still standing by the window, gazing with
pleasure at the kids playing baseball below. He wheeled around as I opened the door, and his face
assumed a cheery expression.
‘Hiya, Doc!’ he said. ‘Nice morning! You want to talk to me— do I take this chair here?’ There was
no sign of recognition on his frank, open face.
‘Haven’t we met before, Mr G.?’ I asked casually.
‘No, I can’t say we have. Quite a beard you got there. I wouldn’t forget you, Doc!’
‘Why do you call me “Doc”?’
‘Well, you are a doc, ain’t you?’
‘Yes, but if you haven’t met me, how do you know what I am?’
‘You talk like a doc. I can see you’re a doc’
‘Well, you’re right, I am. I’m the neurologist here.’
‘Neurologist? Hey, there’s something wrong with my nerves? And “here”—where’s “here”? What is
this place anyhow?’
‘I was just going to ask you—where do you think you are?’
‘I see these beds, and these patients everywhere. Looks like a sort of hospital to me. But hell, what
would I be doing in a hospital—and with all these old people, years older than me. I feel good, I’m
strong as a bull. Maybe I work here ... Do I work? What’s my job? ... No, you’re shaking your head, I
see in your eyes I don’t work here. If I don’t work here, I’ve been put here. Am I a patient, am I sick and
don’t know it, Doc? It’s crazy, it’s scary ... Is it some sort of joke?’
‘You don’t know what the matter is? You really don’t know? You remember telling me about your
childhood, growing up in Connecticut, working as a radio operator on submarines? And how your
brother is engaged to a girl from Oregon?’
‘Hey, you’re right. But I didn’t tell you that, I never met you before in my life. You must have read
all about me in my chart.
‘Okay,’ I said. ‘I’ll tell you a story. A man went to his doctor complaining of memory lapses. The
doctor asked him some routine questions, and then said, “These lapses. What about them?” “What
lapses?” the patient replied.’
‘So that’s my problem,’ Jimmie laughed. ‘I kinda thought it was. I do find myself forgetting things,
once in a while—things that have just happened. The past is clear, though.’
‘Will you allow me to examine you, to run over some tests?’
‘Sure,’ he said genially. ‘Whatever you want.’
On intelligence testing he showed excellent ability. He was quick-witted, observant, and logical, and
had no difficulty solving complex problems and puzzles—no difficulty, that is, if they could be done
quickly. If much time was required, he forgot what he was doing. He was quick and good at tic-tac-toe
and checkers, and cunning and aggressive—he easily beat me. But he got lost at chess—the moves were
too slow.
Homing in on his memory, I found an extreme and extraordinary loss of recent memory—so that
whatever was said or shown to him was apt to be forgotten in a few seconds’ time. Thus I laid out my
watch, my tie, and my glasses on the desk, covered them, and asked him to remember these. Then, after
a minute’s chat, I asked him what I had put under the cover. He remembered none of them—or indeed
that I had even asked him to remember. I repeated the test, this time getting him to write down the
names of the three objects; again he forgot, and when I showed him the paper with his writing on it he
was astounded, and said he had no recollection of writing anything down, though he acknowledged that
it was his own writing, and then got a faint ‘echo’ of the fact that he had written them down.
He sometimes retained faint memories, some dim echo or sense of familiarity. Thus five minutes
after I had played tic-tac-toe with him, he recollected that ‘some doctor’ had played this with him ‘a
while back’—whether the ‘while back’ was minutes or months ago he had no idea. He then paused and
said, ‘It could have been you?’ When I said it was me, he seemed amused. This faint amusement and
indifference were very characteristic, as were the involved cogitations to which he was driven by being
so disoriented and lost in time. When I asked Jimmie the time of the year, he would immediately look
around for some clue—I was careful to remove the calendar from my desk—and would work out the
time of year, roughly, by looking through the window.
It was not, apparently, that he failed to register in memory, but that the memory traces were fugitive
in the extreme, and were apt to be effaced within a minute, often less, especially if there were distracting
or competing stimuli, while his intellectual and perceptual powers were preserved, and highly superior.
Jimmie’s scientific knowledge was that of a bright high school graduate with a penchant for
mathematics and science. He was superb at arithmetical (and also algebraic) calculations, but only if
they could be done with lightning speed. If there were many steps, too much time, involved, he would
forget where he was, and even the question. He knew the elements, compared them, and drew the
periodic table—but omitted the transuranic elements.
‘Is that complete?’ I asked when he’d finished.
‘It’s complete and up-to-date, sir, as far as I know.’
‘You wouldn’t know any elements beyond uranium?’
‘You kidding? There’s ninety-two elements, and uranium’s the last.’
I paused and flipped through a National Geographic on the table. ‘Tell me the planets,’ I said, ‘and
something about them.’ Unhesitatingly, confidently, he gave me the planets—their names, their
discovery, their distance from the sun, their estimated mass, character, and gravity.
‘What is this?’ I asked, showing him a photo in the magazine I was holding.
‘It’s the moon,’ he replied.
‘No, it’s not,’ I answered. ‘It’s a picture of the earth taken from the moon.’
‘Doc, you’re kidding! Someone would’ve had to get a camera up there!’
‘Naturally.’
‘Hell! You’re joking—how the hell would you do that?’
Unless he was a consummate actor, a fraud simulating an astonishment he did not feel, this was an
utterly convincing demonstration that he was still in the past. His words, his feelings, his innocent
wonder, his struggle to make sense of what he saw, were precisely those of an intelligent young man in
the forties faced with the future, with what had not yet happened, and what was scarcely imaginable.
‘This more than anything else,’ I wrote in my notes, ‘persuades me that his cut-off around 1945 is
genuine ... What I showed him, and told him, produced the authentic amazement which it would have
done in an intelligent young man of the pre-Sputnik era.’
I found another photo in the magazine and pushed it over to him.
‘That’s an aircraft carrier,’ he said. ‘Real ultramodern design. I never saw one quite like that.’
‘What’s it called?’ I asked.
He glanced down, looked baffled, and said, ‘The Nimitzl’
‘Something the matter?’
‘The hell there is!’ he replied hotly. ‘I know ‘em all by name, and I don’t know a Nimitz ... Of course
there’s an Admiral Nimitz, but I never heard they named a carrier after him.’
Angrily he threw the magazine down.
He was becoming fatigued, and somewhat irritable and anxious, under the continuing pressure of
anomaly and contradiction, and their fearful implications, to which he could not be entirely oblivious. I
had already, unthinkingly, pushed him into panic, and felt it was time to end our session. We wandered
over to the window again, and looked down at the sunlit baseball diamond; as he looked his face
relaxed, he forgot the Nimitz, the satellite photo, the other horrors and hints, and became absorbed in the
game below. Then, as a savory smell drifted up from the dining room, he smacked his lips, said
‘Lunch!’, smiled, and took his leave.
And I myself was wrung with emotion—it was heartbreaking, it was absurd, it was deeply
perplexing, to think of his life lost in limbo, dissolving.
‘He is, as it were,’ I wrote in my notes, ‘isolated in a single moment of being, with a moat or lacuna
of forgetting all round him ... He is man without a past (or future), stuck in a constantly changing,
meaningless moment.’ And then, more prosaically, ‘The remainder of the neurological examination is
entirely normal. Impression: probably Korsakov’s syndrome, due to alcoholic degeneration of the
mammillary bodies.’ My note was a strange mixture of facts and observations, carefully noted and
itemized, with irrepressible meditations on what such problems might ‘mean’, in regard to who and what
and where this poor man was—whether, indeed, one could speak of an ‘existence’, given so absolute a
privation of memory or continuity.
I kept wondering, in this and later notes—unscientifically— about ‘a lost soul’, and how one might
establish some continuity, some roots, for he was a man without roots, or rooted only in the remote past.
‘Only connect’—but how could he connect, and how could we help him to connect? What was life
without connection? ‘I may venture to affirm,’ Hume wrote, ‘that we are nothing but a bundle or
collection of different sensations, which succeed each other with an inconceivable rapidity, and are in a
perpetual flux and movement.’ In some sense, he had been reduced to a ‘Humean’ being— I could not
help thinking how fascinated Hume would have been at seeing in Jimmie his own philosophical
‘chimaera’ incarnate, a gruesome reduction of a man to mere disconnected, incoherent flux and change.
Perhaps I could find advice or help in the medical literature— a literature which, for some reason,
was largely Russian, from Korsakov’s original thesis (Moscow, 1887) about such cases of memory loss,
which are still called ‘Korsakov’s syndrome’, to Luria’s Neuropsychology of Memory (which appeared
in translation only a year after I first saw Jimmie). Korsakov wrote in 1887:
Memory of recent events is disturbed almost exclusively; recent impressions apparently disappear
soonest, whereas impressions of long ago are recalled properly, so that the patient’s ingenuity, his
sharpness of wit, and his resourcefulness remain largely unaffected.
To Korsakov’s brilliant but spare observations, almost a century of further research has been added—
the richest and deepest, by far, being Luria’s. And in Luria’s account science became poetry, and the
pathos of radical lostness was evoked. ‘Gross disturbances of the organization of impressions of events
and their sequence in time can always be observed in such patients,’ he wrote. ‘In consequence, they
lose their integral experience of time and begin to live in a world of isolated impressions.’ Further, as
Luria noted, the eradication of impressions (and their disorder) might spread backward in time—’in the
most serious cases—even to relatively distant events.’
Most of Luria’s patients, as described in this book, had massive and serious cerebral tumors, which
had the same effects as Korsakov’s syndrome, but later spread and were often fatal. Luria included no
cases of ‘simple’ Korsakov’s syndrome, based on the self-limiting destruction that Korsakov
described—neuron destruction, produced by alcohol, in the tiny but crucial mammillary bodies, the rest
of the brain being perfectly preserved. And so there was no long-term follow-up of Luria’s cases.
I had at first been deeply puzzled, and dubious, even suspicious, about the apparently sharp cut-off in
1945, a point, a date, which was also symbolically so sharp. I wrote in a subsequent note:
There is a great blank. We do not know what happened then— or subsequently ... We must fill in
these ‘missing’ years— from his brother, or the navy, or hospitals he has been to ... Could it be that he
sustained some massive trauma at this time, some massive cerebral or emotional trauma in combat, in
the war, and that this may have affected him ever since? ... was the war his ‘high point’, the last time he
was really alive, and existence since one long anti-climax? (In his fascinating oral history The Good
War (1985) Studs Terkel transcribes countless stories of men and women, especially fighting men, who
felt World War II was intensely real—by far the most real and significant time of their lives—everything
since as pallid in comparison. Such men tend to dwell on the war and to relive its battles, comradeship,
moral certainties and intensity. But this dwelling on the past and relative hebetude towards the
present—this emotional dulling of current feeling and memory—is nothing like Jimmie’s organic
amnesia. I recently had occasion to discuss the question with Terkel: ‘I’ve met thousands of men,’ he
told me, ‘who feel they’ve just been “marking time” since ‘45—but I never met anyone for whom time
terminated, like your amnesiac Jimmie.’)
We did various tests on him (EEG, brain scans), and found no evidence of massive brain damage,
although atrophy of the tiny mammillary bodies would not show up on such tests. We received reports
from the navy indicating that he had remained in the navy until 1965, and that he was perfectly
competent at that time.
Then we turned up a short nasty report from Bellevue Hospital, dated 1971, saying that he was
‘totally disoriented ... with an advanced organic brain syndrome, due to alcohol’ (cirrhosis had also
developed by this time). From Bellevue he was sent to a wretched dump in the Village, a so-called
‘nursing home’ whence he was rescued—lousy, starving—by our Home in 1975.
We located his brother, whom Jimmie always spoke of as being in accountancy school and engaged
to a girl from Oregon. In fact he had married the girl from Oregon, had become a father and grandfather,
and been a practicing accountant for thirty years.
Where we had hoped for an abundance of information and feeling from his brother, we received a
courteous but somewhat meager letter. It was obvious from reading this—especially reading between the
lines—that the brothers had scarcely seen each other since 1943, and gone separate ways, partly through
the vicissitudes of location and profession, and partly through deep (though not estranging) differences
of temperament. Jimmie, it seemed, had never ‘settled down’, was ‘happy-go-lucky’, and ‘always a
drinker’. The navy, his brother felt, provided a structure, a life, and the real problems started when he
left it, in 1965. Without his habitual structure and anchor Jimmie had ceased to work, ‘gone to pieces,’
and started to drink heavily. There had been some memory impairment, of the Korsakov type, in the
middle and especially the late Sixties, but not so severe that Jimmie couldn’t ‘cope’ in his nonchalant
fashion. But his drinking grew heavier in 1970.
Around Christmas of that year, his brother understood, he had suddenly ‘blown his top’ and become
deliriously excited and confused, and it was at this point he had been taken into Bellevue. During the
next month, the excitement and delirium died down, but he was left with deep and bizarre memory
lapses, or ‘deficits,’ to use the medical jargon. His brother had visited him at this time—they had not
met for twenty years—and, to his horror, Jimmie not only failed to recognize him, but said, ‘Stop
joking! You’re old enough to be my father. My brother’s a young man, just going through accountancy
school.’
When I received this information, I was more perplexed still: why did Jimmie not remember his later
years in the navy, why did he not recall and organize his memories until 1970? I had not heard then that
such patients might have a retrograde amnesia (see Postscript). ‘I wonder, increasingly,’ I wrote at this
time, ‘whether there is not an element of hysterical or fugal amnesia—whether he is not in flight from
something too awful to recall’, and I suggested he be seen by our psychiatrist. Her report was searching
and detailed—the examination had included a sodium amytal test, calculated to ‘release’ any memories
which might be repressed.
She also attempted to hypnotize Jimmie, in the hope of eliciting memories repressed by hysteria—
this tends to work well in cases of hysterical amnesia. But it failed because Jimmie could not be
hypnotized, not because of any ‘resistance,’ but because of his extreme amnesia, which caused him to
lose track of what the hypnotist was saying. (Dr M. Homonoff, who worked on the amnesia ward at the
Boston Veterans Administration hospital, tells me of similar experiences—and of his feeling that this is
absolutely characteristic of patients with Korsakov’s, as opposed to patients with hysterical amnesia.)
‘I have no feeling or evidence,’ the psychiatrist wrote, ‘of any hysterical or “put-on” deficit. He lacks
both the means and the motive to make a facade. His memory deficits are organic and permanent and
incorrigible, though it is puzzling they should go back so long.’ Since, she felt, he was ‘unconcerned ...
manifested no special anxiety ... constituted no management problem,’ there was nothing she could
offer, or any therapeutic ‘entrance’ or ‘lever’ she could see.
At this point, persuaded that this was, indeed, ‘pure’ Korsakov’s, uncomplicated by other factors,
emotional or organic, I wrote to Luria and asked his opinion. He spoke in his reply of his patient Bel,
whose amnesia had retroactively eradicated ten years. He said he saw no reason why such a retrograde
amnesia should not thrust backward decades, or almost a whole lifetime. ‘I can only wait for the final
amnesia,’ Buriuel writes, ‘the one that can erase an entire life.’ But Jimmies amnesia, for whatever
reason, had erased memory and time back to 1945—roughly—and then stopped. Occasionally, he would
recall something much later, but the recall was fragmentary and dislocated in time. Once, seeing the
word ‘satellite’ in a newspaper headline, he said offhandedly that he’d been involved in a project of
satellite tracking while on the ship Chesapeake Bay, a memory fragment coming from the early or midSixties. But, for all practical purposes, his cut-off point was during the mid- (or late) Forties, and
anything subsequently retrieved was fragmentary, unconnected. I his was the case in 1975, and it is still
the case now, nine years later.
What could we do? What should we do? There are no prescriptions,’ Luria wrote, ‘in a case like this.
Do whatever your ingenuity and your heart suggest. There is little or no hope of any recovery in his
memory. But a man does not consist of memory alone. He has feeling, will, sensibilities, moral being—
matters of which neuropsychology cannot speak. And it is here, beyond the realm of an impersonal
psychology, that you may find ways to touch him, and change him. And the circumstances of your work
especially allow this, for you work in a Home, which is like a little world, quite different from the clinics
and institutions where I work. Neuropsychological!}’, there is little or nothing you can do; but in the
realm of the Individual, there may be much you can do.’
Luria mentioned his patient Kur as manifesting a rare self-awareness, in which hopelessness was
mixed with an odd equanimity. ‘I have no memory of the present,’ Kur would say. ‘I do not know what I
have just done or from where I have just come ... I can recall my past very well, but I have no memory
of my present.’ When asked whether he had ever seen the person testing him, he said, ‘I cannot say yes
or no, I can neither affirm nor deny that I have seen you.’ This was sometimes the case with Jimmie;
and, like Kur, who stayed many months in the same hospital, Jimmie began to form ‘a sense of
familiarity’; he slowly learned his way around the home—the whereabouts of the dining room, his own
room, the elevators, the stairs, and in some sense recognized some of the staff, although he confused
them, and perhaps had to do so, with people from the past. He soon became fond of the nursing sister in
the Home; he recognized her voice, her footfalls, immediately, but would always say that she had been a
fellow pupil at his high school, and was greatly surprised when I addressed her as ‘Sister’.
‘Gee!’ he exclaimed, ‘the damnedest things happen. I’d never have guessed you’d become a
religious, Sister!’
Since he’s been at our Home—that is, since early 1975—Jimmie has never been able to identify
anyone in it consistently. The only person he truly recognizes is his brother, whenever he visits from
Oregon. These meetings are deeply emotional and moving to observe—the only truly emotional
meetings Jimmie has. He loves his brother, he recognizes him, but he cannot understand why he looks so
old: ‘Guess some people age fast,’ he says. Actually his brother looks much younger than his age, and
has the sort of face and build that change little with the years. These are true meetings, Jimmie’s only
connection of past and present, yet they do nothing to provide any sense of history or continuity. If
anything they emphasize—at least to his brother, and to others who see them together—that Jimmie still
lives, is fossilized, in the past.
All of us, at first, had high hopes of helping Jimmie—he was so personable, so likable, so quick and
intelligent, it was difficult to believe that he might be beyond help. But none of us had ever encountered,
even imagined, such a power of amnesia, the possibility of a pit into which everything, every
experience, every event, would fathomlessly drop, a bottomless memory-hole that would engulf the
whole world.
I suggested, when I first saw him, that he should keep a diary, and be encouraged to keep notes every
day of his experiences, his feelings, thoughts, memories, reflections. These attempts were foiled, at first,
by his continually losing the diary: it had to be attached to him—somehow. But this too failed to work:
he dutifully kept a brief daily notebook but could not recognize his earlier entries in it. He does
recognize his own writing, and style, and is always astounded to find that he wrote something the day
before.
Astounded—and indifferent—for he was a man who, in effect, had no ‘day before’. His entries
remained unconnected and un-connecting and had no power to provide any sense of time or continuity.
Moreover, they were trivial—’Eggs for breakfast’, ‘Watched ballgame on TV—and never touched the
depths. But were there depths in this unmemoried man, depths of an abiding feeling and thinking, or had
he been reduced to a sort of Humean drivel, a mere succession of unrelated impressions and events?
Jimmie both was and wasn’t aware of this deep, tragic loss in himself, loss of himself. (If a man has
lost a leg or an eye, he knows he has lost a leg or an eye; but if he has lost a self—himself—he cannot
know it, because he is no longer there to know it.) Therefore I could not question him intellectually
about such matters.
He had originally professed bewilderment at finding himself amid patients, when, as he said, he
himself didn’t feel ill. But what, we wondered, did he feel? He was strongly built and fit, he had a sort of
animal strength and energy, but also a strange inertia, passivity, and (as everyone remarked)
‘unconcern’; he gave all of us an overwhelming sense of ‘something missing,’ although this, if he
realized it, was itself accepted with an odd ‘unconcern.’ One day I asked him not about his memory, or
past, but about the simplest and most elemental feelings of all:
‘How do you feel?’
‘How do I feel,’ he repeated, and scratched his head. ‘I cannot say I feel ill. But I cannot say I feel
well. I cannot say I feel anything at all.’
‘Are you miserable?’ I continued.
‘Can’t say I am.’
‘Do you enjoy life?’
‘I can’t say I do ... ‘
I hesitated, fearing that I was going too far, that I might be stripping a man down to some hidden,
unacknowledgeable, unbearable despair.
‘You don’t enjoy life,’ I repeated, hesitating somewhat. ‘How then do you feel about life?’
‘I can’t say that I feel anything at all.’
‘You feel alive though?’
‘Feel alive? Not really. I haven’t felt alive for a very long time.’
His face wore a look of infinite sadness and resignation.
Later, having noted his aptitude for, and pleasure in, quick games and puzzles, and their power to
‘hold’ him, at least while they lasted, and to allow, for a while, a sense of companionship and
competition—he had not complained of loneliness, but he looked so alone; he never expressed sadness,
but he looked so sad— I suggested he be brought into our recreation programs at the Home. This
worked better—better than the diary. He would become keenly and briefly involved in games, but soon
they ceased to offer any challenge: he solved all the puzzles, and could solve them easily; and he was far
better and sharper than anyone else at games. And as he found this out, he grew fretful and restless
again, and wandered the corridors, uneasy and bored and with a sense of indignity—games and puzzles
were for children, a diversion. Clearly, passionately, he wanted something to do: he wanted to do, to be,
to feel—and could not; he wanted sense, he wanted purpose—in Freud’s words, ‘Work and Love’.
Could he do ‘ordinary’ work? He had ‘gone to pieces’, his brother said, when he ceased to work in
1965. He had two striking skills— Morse code and touch-typing. We could not use Morse, unless we
invented a use; but good typing we could use, if he could recover his old skills—and this would be real
work, not just a game. Jimmie soon did recover his old skill and came to type very quickly—he could
not do it slowly—and found in this some of the challenge and satisfaction of a job. But still this was
superficial tapping and typing; it was trivial, it did not reach to the depths. And what he typed, he typed
mechanically—he could not hold the thought—the short sentences following one another in a
meaningless order.
One tended to speak of him, instinctively, as a spiritual casualty—a ‘lost soul’: was it possible that he
had really been ‘de-souled’ by a disease? ‘Do you think he has a soul?’ I once asked the Sisters. They
were outraged by my question, but could see why I asked it. ‘Watch Jimmie in chapel,’ they said, ‘and
judge for yourself.’
I did, and I was moved, profoundly moved and impressed, because I saw here an intensity and
steadiness of attention and concentration that I had never seen before in him or conceived him capable
of. I watched him kneel and take the Sacrament on his tongue, and could not doubt the fullness and
totality of Communion, the perfect alignment of his spirit with the spirit of the Mass. Fully, intensely,
quietly, in the quietude of absolute concentration and attention, he entered and partook of the Holy
Communion. He was wholly held, absorbed, by a feeling. There was no forgetting, no Korsakov’s then,
nor did it seem possible or imaginable that there should be; for he was no longer at the mercy of a faulty
and fallible mechanism—that of meaningless sequences and memory traces—but was absorbed in an
act, an act of his whole being, which carried feeling and meaning in an organic continuity and unity, a
continuity and unity so seamless it could not permit any break.
Clearly Jimmie found himself, found continuity and reality, in the absoluteness of spiritual attention
and act. The Sisters were right—he did find his soul here. And so was Luria, whose words now came
back to me: ‘A man does not consist of memory alone. He has feeling, will, sensibility, moral being ... It
is here ... you may touch him, and see a profound change.’ Memory, mental activity, mind alone, could
not hold him; but moral attention and action could hold him completely.
But perhaps ‘moral’ was too narrow a word—for the aesthetic and dramatic were equally involved.
Seeing Jim in the chapel opened my eyes to other realms where the soul is called on, and held, and
stilled, in attention and communion. The same depth of absorption and attention was to be seen in
relation to music and art: he had no difficulty, I noticed, ‘following’ music or simple dramas, for every
moment in music and art refers to, contains, other moments. He liked gardening, and had taken over
some of the work in our garden. At first he greeted the garden each day as new, but for some reason this
had become more familiar to him than the inside of the Home. He almost never got lost or disoriented in
the garden now; he patterned it, I think, on loved and remembered gardens from his youth in
Connecticut.
Jimmie, who was so lost in extensional ‘spatial’ time, was perfectly organized in Bergsonian
‘intentional’ time; what was fugitive, unsustainable, as formal structure, was perfectly stable, perfectly
held, as art or will. Moreover, there was something that endured and survived. If Jimmie was briefly
‘held’ by a task or puzzle or game or calculation, held in the purely mental challenge of these, he would
fall apart as soon as they were done, into the abyss of his nothingness, his amnesia. But if he was held in
emotional and spiritual attention—in the contemplation of nature or art, in listening to music, in taking
part in the Mass in chapel—the attention, its ‘mood’, its quietude, would persist for a while, and there
would be in him a pensiveness and peace we rarely, if ever, saw during the rest of his life at the Home.
I have known Jimmie now for nine years—and neuropsychologically, he has not changed in the least.
He still has the severest, most devastating Korsakov’s, cannot remember isolated items for more than a
few seconds, and has a dense amnesia going back to 1945. But humanly, spiritually, he is at times a
different man altogether—no longer fluttering, restless, bored, and lost, but deeply attentive to the
beauty and soul of the world, rich in all the Kierkegaardian categories—and aesthetic, the moral, the
religious, the dramatic. I had wondered, when I first met him, if he was not condemned to a sort of
‘Humean’ froth, a meaningless fluttering on the surface of life, and whether there was any way of
transcending the incoherence of his Humean disease. Empirical science told me there was not—but
empirical science, empiricism, takes no account of the soul, no account of what constitutes and
determines personal being. Perhaps there is a philosophical as well as a clinical lesson here: that in
Korsakov’s, or dementia, or other such catastrophes, however great the organic damage and Humean
dissolution, there remains the undiminished possibility of reintegration by art, by communion, by
touching the human spirit: and this can be preserved in what seems at first a hopeless state of
neurological devastation.
Postscript
I know now that retrograde amnesia, to some degree, is very common, if not universal, in cases of
Korsakov’s. The classical Korsakov’s syndrome—a profound and permanent, but ‘pure’, devastation of
memory caused by alcoholic destruction of the mammillary bodies— is rare, even among very heavy
drinkers. One may, of course, see Korsakov’s syndrome with other pathologies, as in Luria’s patients
with tumors. A particularly fascinating case of an acute (and mercifully transient) Korsakov’s syndrome
has been well described only very recently in the so-called Transient Global Amnesia (TGA) which may
occur with migraines, head injuries or impaired blood supply to the brain. Here, for a few minutes or
hours, a severe and singular amnesia may occur, even though the patient may continue to drive a car, or,
perhaps, to carry on medical or editorial duties, in a mechanical way. But under this fluency lies a
profound amnesia— every sentence uttered being forgotten as soon as it is said, everything forgotten
within a few minutes of being seen, though long-established memories and routines may be perfectly
preserved. (Some remarkable videotapes of patients during TGAs have recently [1986] been made by Dr
John Hodges, of Oxford.)
Further, there may be a profound retrograde amnesia in such cases. My colleague Dr. Leon Protass
tells me of a case seen by him recently, in which a highly intelligent man was unable for some hours to
remember his wife or children, to remember that he had a wife or children. In effect, he lost thirty years
of his life— though, fortunately, for only a few hours. Recovery from such attacks is prompt and
complete—yet they are, in a sense, the most horrifying of ‘little strokes’ in their power absolutely to
annul or obliterate decades of richly lived, richly achieving, richly memoried life. The horror, typically,
is only felt by others—the patient, unaware, amnesiac for his amnesia, may continue what he is doing,
quite unconcerned, and only discover later that he lost not only a day (as is common with ordinary
alcoholic ‘blackouts’), but half a lifetime, and never knew it. The fact that one can lose the greater part
of a lifetime has peculiar, uncanny horror.
In adulthood, life, higher life, may be brought to a premature end by strokes, senility, brain injuries,
etc., but there usually remains the consciousness of life lived, of one’s past. This is usually felt as a sort
of compensation: ‘At least I lived fully, tasting life to the full, before I was brain-injured, stricken, etc.’
This sense of ‘the life lived before’, which may be either a consolation or a torment, is precisely what is
taken away in retrograde amnesia. The ‘final amnesia, the one that can erase an entire life’ that Bunuel
speaks of may occur, perhaps, in a terminal dementia, but not, in my experience, suddenly, in
consequence of a stroke. But there is a different, yet comparable, sort of amnesia, which can occur
suddenly—different in that it is not ‘global’ but ‘modality-specific’.
Thus, in one patient under my care, a sudden thrombosis in the posterior circulation of the brain
caused the immediate death of the visual parts of the brain. Forthwith this patient became completely
blind—but did not know it. He looked blind—but he made no complaints. Questioning and testing
showed, beyond doubt, that not only was he centrally or ‘cortically’ blind, but he had lost all visual
images and memories, lost them totally—yet had no sense of any loss. Indeed, he had lost the very idea
of seeing—and was not only unable to describe anything visually, but bewildered when I used words
such as ‘seeing’ and ‘light.’ He had become, in essence, a non-visual being. His entire lifetime of seeing,
of visuality, had, in effect, been stolen. His whole visual life had, indeed, been erased—and erased